by Laura Damone Tue Nov 17, 2020 5:54 pm
Great question, Barry!
Yes, the order of the correlated items in an answer impacts the extent to which the answer strengthens the argument.
In this argument, we've got 100 kids. Of those kids, some showed the most improvement in their composition skills: we'll call them group C. There are also kids that learned to write their letters most automatically. We'll call them group A.
The premise of the argument tells us that everyone in group C is also in group A. The conclusion posits that being in group A is the cause of getting into group C.
This type of causal conclusion, the kind that posits that one thing is the cause of another thing, is a vulnerable claim. You can attack this claim by showing either type of counterexample: the cause without the effect or the effect without the cause.
In this argument, our premise tells us that effect without cause is impossible: Everybody in group C is also in group A. But what if there were a whole bunch of A's that weren't C's? If the cause frequently existed without leading to the effect, that would weaken the causal conclusion. So to strengthen this argument, we can rule out the possibility of those instances of the cause without the effect by establishing that the A's are generally C's. That's why answer choice C is the credited answer.
In your post, you mentioned superlatives and gradients. Let's talk about superlatives first. Those are actually a distraction in this question. Answer choice C isn't correct because it shows that the correlation still holds when you downgrade the superlative from "the most" to "greater," though I see why you thought that! Answer choice C is correct because it shows that the correlation holds in both directions. The premise tells us that the effect is correlated with the cause 100% of the time: all the students in group C were also in group A. Answer choice C tells us that the cause is generally correlated with the effect: The students in group A are generally in group C. As stated above, this strengthens the argument by ruling out the possibility of a bunch of "cause without effect" counterexamples that would drag our argument down.
Now, lets talk about gradients. There are two specific types of gradient comparisons to be on the lookout for: proportional relationships and inversely proportional relationships. In a proportional relationship, a change in one thing is associated with a similar change in a second thing. As one goes up, so goes the other. As one goes down, so goes the other. In an inversely proportional relationship, a change in one thing is associated with the opposite change in a second thing. As one goes up, the other goes down, and vica versa.
Answer choice C doesn't actually express either type of gradient. It almost expresses a proportional relationship, but it falls just short because it doesn't actually guarantee that change in one thing is associated with change in the other. The word "generally" is a big red flag here. Yes, the answer tells us that greater improvement in automatic letter-writing is generally correlated with greater improvement in composition. But it doesn't actually tell us that as letter writing becomes more automatic, composition improves. It shows a point of correlation, but it doesn't show a pattern of proportional correlation that gets us to that point.
I hope this helps! There's a lot to unpack there, so let me know if you have additional questions.
Laura Damone
LSAT Content & Curriculum Lead | Manhattan Prep